Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 36, Issue 3, 1 July 2007, Pages 497-510
NeuroImage

Towards a nosology for frontotemporal lobar degenerations—A meta-analysis involving 267 subjects

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.024Get rights and content

Abstract

Frontotemporal lobar degeneration is the second most common diagnosis of dementia in individuals younger than 65 years. We conducted a systematic and quantitative meta-analysis to examine neural correlates of frontotemporal lobar degeneration and its subtypes and to place the disease in a framework of cognitive neuropsychiatry. MedLine and Current Contents search engines were used to identify functional and anatomical imaging studies investigating frontotemporal lobar degeneration between 1980 and 2005. Studies were included, if they were peer-reviewed, applied internationally recognized diagnostic criteria, were original studies, and had results normalized to a stereotactic space. 19 studies were identified reporting either atrophy or decreases in glucose utilization. Finally, the analysis involved 267 subjects suffering from frontotemporal lobar degeneration and 351 control subjects. A quantitative meta-analysis was performed. Maxima of the studies resulted in activation likelihood estimates. The meta-analysis revealed specific neural networks for each of the three clinically defined subtypes of frontotemporal lobar degeneration, namely frontotemporal dementia, semantic dementia, and progressive non-fluent aphasia. Networks did not overlap as shown by a conjunction analysis, and they corresponded to clinical characteristics. The study relates the clinical features of each subtype of frontotemporal lobar degeneration specifically to its neural substrate. By ‘triple dissociating’ frontotemporal lobar degenerations into three clinicoanatomical prototypes, the study contributes to placing these disorders in cognitive neuropsychiatry and suggests a respective nosology.

Introduction

Recent studies suggest that frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD) is the second most common diagnosis of dementia in individuals younger than 65 years (Johnson et al., 2005). Although the size of FTLD cohorts is modest at any one center, the recent establishment of consensus criteria for FTLD (Clinical and Neuropathological Criteria for Frontotemporal Dementia by the Lund and Manchester groups, 1994, Neary et al., 1998) represents an opportunity to begin large-scale studies. These criteria (Neary et al., 1998) divide FTLD into three major subgroups: frontotemporal dementia (FTD), semantic dementia (SD), and progressive non-fluent aphasia (PNFA). FTD is characterized by alterations in behavior and personality, namely by decline in social interpersonal conduct, impairment in regulation of personal conduct, emotional blunting, and loss of insight. Patients meet the criteria for SD if they exhibit a language disorder characterized by the following: empty fluent speech, loss of word meaning, or semantic paraphasias, a perceptual disorder characterized by impaired recognition of familiar faces or object identity, preserved perceptual matching and drawing reproduction, preserved single-word repetition, and preserved ability to read aloud and write to dictation orthographically regular words. Finally, patients meet the criteria for PNFA if they have non-fluent spontaneous speech with at least one of the following: agrammatism, phonemic paraphasias, or anomia. Behavioral changes are almost absent. Besides the clinical criteria, neuropsychological and brain imaging criteria have been proposed by Neary et al. (1998). According to these criteria, FTD is characterized by bilateral impairment in the (pre-) frontal and anterior temporal brain regions, PNFA by mainly left-sided alterations in the same areas, and SD by changes in the anterior temporal lobe greater on the left than on the right side.

The clinical criteria for FTLD represent a first step toward understanding neurodegenerative disorders that affect the frontal and temporal lobes. However, neural deficits seem to overlap in the different subtypes of FTLD (Neary et al., 1998). Accordingly, one aim of our study was to analyze neural substrates of FTLD and its subtypes and to investigate their specificity in particular. Because FTLD subtypes can be clinically dissociated (Neary et al., 1998), we also hypothesized a dissociation of the neural networks involved. Moreover, we wanted to contribute to a cognitive neuropsychiatry of FTLD by relating clinical deficits to their neural substrates (Halligan and David, 2001) as previously shown for one subtype, namely FTD (Schroeter et al., 2007). Because recently introduced diagnostic criteria enable comparisons of cohorts from different centers (Clinical and Neuropathological Criteria for Frontotemporal Dementia by the Lund and Manchester groups, 1994, Neary et al., 1998), we applied a systematic meta-analytic approach. The meta-analysis included morphometric studies investigating brain atrophy with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and functional imaging studies measuring reduction in glucose utilization by (18F) fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography (FDG-PET). We chose the method developed by Turkeltaub et al. (2002), since it enables a quantitative meta-analytic approach, is considered as the most sophisticated and well-validated of coordinate-based voxel-wise meta-analyses (Fox et al., 2005), and has been recently introduced to studies on morphometry and glucose utilization (Schroeter et al., 2007).

Section snippets

General study selection criteria

MedLine and Current Contents search engines were used to identify studies on morphometry and glucose utilization in FTLD and its three subtypes. The following search strategy was applied: ((progressive AND aphasia) OR (frontotemporal AND dementia) OR Pick OR (semantic AND dementia)) AND (PET OR MRT OR MRI). Studies were included if they fulfilled the following criteria: (1) published between January 1980 and October 2005, (2) peer-reviewed, (3) diagnosis according to internationally recognized

Results

Fig. 1 and the appendix show the maxima of the several studies, which were included in the meta-analysis. For each study all maxima were taken into account avoiding any a priori assumptions. The results of the quantitative meta-analyses are summarized in Table 3. Fig. 2 displays above-threshold voxels on an individual brain in Talairach space for the different FTLD patient groups with pooled studies applying FDG-PET and MRI. For FTD seven significant above-threshold clusters were identified.

Discussion

Our systematic and quantitative meta-analytic approach ‘triple dissociates’ FTLD by showing that, besides a slight potential overlap in the subcallosal region for FTD and SD, each of its three subtypes involves a specific neural network. Correspondingly, diagnostic criteria for FTLD create a ‘triple dissociation’ of these subtypes with a high diagnostic accuracy clinically (Clinical and Neuropathological Criteria for Frontotemporal Dementia by the Lund and Manchester groups, 1994, Neary et al.,

References (99)

  • J.R. Hodges et al.

    Semantic memory disorders

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (1997)
  • B. Ibach et al.

    Contrasting metabolic impairment in frontotemporal degeneration and early onset Alzheimer's disease

    NeuroImage

    (2004)
  • T. Jacobsen et al.

    Brain correlates of aesthetic judgment of beauty

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • J. Keane et al.

    Face and emotion processing in frontal variant frontotemporal dementia

    Neuropsychologia

    (2002)
  • S. Lough et al.

    Measuring and modifying abnormal social cognition in frontal variant frontotemporal dementia

    J. Psychosom. Res.

    (2002)
  • S. Lough et al.

    Social reasoning, emotion and empathy in frontotemporal dementia

    Neuropsychologia

    (2006)
  • A. Martin et al.

    Semantic memory and the brain: structure and processes

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2001)
  • M.F. Mendez

    What frontotemporal dementia reveals about the neurobiological basis of morality

    Med. Hypotheses

    (2006)
  • S. Nakano et al.

    Relationship between antisocial behavior and regional cerebral blood flow in frontotemporal dementia

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • D. Neary et al.

    Frontotemporal dementia

    Lancet Neurol.

    (2005)
  • G. Northoff et al.

    Cortical midline structures and the self

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2004)
  • G. Northoff et al.

    Self-referential processing in our brain — a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • M.P. Paulus et al.

    Increased activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related to harm avoidance and neuroticism

    NeuroImage

    (2003)
  • S. Rahman et al.

    Decision making and neuropsychiatry

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2001)
  • E. Salmon et al.

    Predominant ventromedial frontopolar metabolic impairment in frontotemporal dementia

    NeuroImage

    (2003)
  • E.J.A. Scherder et al.

    Pain processing in dementia and its relation to neuropathology

    Lancet Neurol.

    (2003)
  • J.S. Snowden et al.

    Social cognition in frontotemporal dementia and Huntington's disease

    Neuropsychologia

    (2003)
  • P.E. Turkeltaub et al.

    Meta-analysis of the functional neuroanatomy of single-word reading: method and validation

    NeuroImage

    (2002)
  • L.K. Tyler et al.

    Towards a distributed account of conceptual knowledge

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2001)
  • G.B. Williams et al.

    Neural correlates of semantic and behavioural deficits in frontotemporal dementia

    NeuroImage

    (2005)
  • J. Xu et al.

    Language in context: emergent features of word, sentence, and narrative comprehension

    NeuroImage

    (2005)
  • S. Zysset et al.

    The anterior frontomedian cortex and evaluative judgment: an fMRI study

    NeuroImage

    (2002)
  • A.L.R. Adlam et al.

    Semantic dementia and fluent primary progressive aphasia: two sides of the same coin?

    Brain

    (2006)
  • R. Adolphs et al.

    Impaired recognition of emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala

    Nature

    (1994)
  • R. Adolphs et al.

    The human amygdala in social judgment

    Nature

    (1998)
  • R. Adolphs et al.

    A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage

    Nature

    (2005)
  • S.W. Anderson et al.

    Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (1999)
  • D. Bathgate et al.

    Behaviour in frontotemporal dementia, Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia

    Acta Neurol. Scand.

    (2001)
  • A.L. Boxer et al.

    Cinguloparietal atrophy distinguishes Alzheimer disease from semantic dementia

    Arch. Neurol.

    (2003)
  • C.S. Carter et al.

    Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance

    Science

    (1998)
  • K. Christoff et al.

    The frontopolar cortex and human cognition: evidence for a rostrocaudal hierarchical organization within the human prefrontal cortex

    Psychobiology

    (2000)
  • J. Neurol., Neurosurg. Psychiatry

    (1994)
  • A.R. Damasio

    The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London B.

    (1996)
  • J. Derrfuss et al.

    Involvement of the inferior frontal junction in cognitive control: meta-analyses of switching and Stroop studies

    Hum. Brain Mapp.

    (2005)
  • P.J. Eslinger et al.

    Metacognitive deficits in frontotemporal dementia

    J. Neurol., Neurosurg. Psychiatry

    (2005)
  • P.T. Fox et al.

    Coordinate-based voxel-wise meta-analysis: dividends of spatial normalization. Report of a virtual workshop

    Hum. Brain Mapp.

    (2005)
  • M. Franceschi et al.

    Glucose metabolism and serotonin receptors in the frontotemporal lobe degeneration

    Ann. Neurol.

    (2005)
  • U. Frith et al.

    Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. London, B

    (2003)
  • P. Garrard et al.

    Semantic dementia: clinical, radiological and pathological perspectives

    J. Neurol.

    (2000)
  • Cited by (139)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text